dc.contributor |
Institut Jean Nicod, CNRS, Paris and Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor |
|
dc.contributor |
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia |
|
dc.creator |
Norenzayan, Ara |
|
dc.creator |
Atran, Scott |
|
dc.creator |
Faulkner, Jason |
|
dc.creator |
Schaller, Mark |
|
dc.date |
2013-06-18T18:32:27Z |
|
dc.date |
2013-06-18T18:32:27Z |
|
dc.date |
2006-05-06 |
|
dc.date.accessioned |
2022-05-19T10:35:07Z |
|
dc.date.available |
2022-05-19T10:35:07Z |
|
dc.identifier |
Norenzayan, Ara; Atran, Scott; Faulkner, Jason; Schaller, Mark (2006). "Memory and Mystery: The Cultural Selection of Minimally Counterintuitive Narratives." Cognitive Science 30(3). <http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/98200> |
|
dc.identifier |
0364-0213 |
|
dc.identifier |
1551-6709 |
|
dc.identifier |
https://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/98200 |
|
dc.identifier |
21702824 |
|
dc.identifier |
10.1207/s15516709cog0000_68 |
|
dc.identifier |
Cognitive Science |
|
dc.identifier |
Lillard, A. S. ( 1998 ). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 1, 3 – 32. |
|
dc.identifier |
Kintsch, W. ( 1994 ). Text comprehension, memory, and learning. American Psychologist, 49, 294 – 303. |
|
dc.identifier |
Kintsch, W., & Greene, L. ( 1978 ). The role of culture‐specific schemata in the comprehension and recall of stories, Discourse Processes, 1, 1 – 13. |
|
dc.identifier |
Koriat, A., Goldsmith, M., & Pansky, A. ( 2000 ). Towards a psychology of memory accuracy. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 481 – 537. |
|
dc.identifier |
Lawson, E. T., & McCauley, R. ( 1990 ) Rethinking religion. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Leslie, A. M. ( 1982 ). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11, 173 – 186. |
|
dc.identifier |
Manheim, R. ( 1977 ). Grimms' tales for young and old: The complete stories. R. Manheim (Trans.). Garden City, NY: Doubleday. (Original work published 1857). |
|
dc.identifier |
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. ( 1990 ). What makes a narrative memorable Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 73 – 82. |
|
dc.identifier |
McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N., & Kintsch, W. ( 1996 ). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 1 – 43. |
|
dc.identifier |
McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. ( 1996 ). Learning from texts: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence. Discourse Processes, 22, 247 – 288. |
|
dc.identifier |
Medin, D. L., & Atran, S. (Eds.). ( 1999 ). Folkbiology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Norenzayan, A., & Atran, S. ( 2004 ). Cognitive and emotional processes in the cultural transmission of natural and nonnatural beliefs. In M. Schaller & C. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture. (pp. 149 – 169 ). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. |
|
dc.identifier |
Norenzayan, A., & Atran, S. ( 2005 ). [Recall of intuitive and counterintuitive ideas in two cultures]. Unpublished raw data. |
|
dc.identifier |
Norenzayan, A., & Hansen, I. ( 2006 ). Belief in supernatural agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 174 – 187. |
|
dc.identifier |
Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. ( 1998 ). Cross‐cultural similarities and differences in the understanding of physical causality. In M. Shield (Ed.), Proceedings of the Seventh Interdisciplinary Conference on Science and Culture (pp. 10 – 21 ). Frankfort, KY: Kentucky State University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Pyysiäinen, I., & Antonnen, V. (Eds.). ( 2002 ). Current approaches in the cognitive science of religion. New York: Continuum. |
|
dc.identifier |
Rosch, E., Mervis, C., Grey, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes‐Braem, P. ( 1976 ). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382 – 439. |
|
dc.identifier |
Rubin, D. C. ( 1995 ). Memory in oral traditions. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Spelke, E. S. ( 1990 ). Principles of object perception. Cognitive Science, 14, 29 – 56. |
|
dc.identifier |
Sperber, D. ( 1975 ). Rethinking symbolism. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Sperber, D. ( 1996 ). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford, England: Blackwell. |
|
dc.identifier |
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. ( 1986 ). Relevance: Communication and cognition ( 2nd Ed. ). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell. |
|
dc.identifier |
Stangor, C., & McMillan, D. ( 1992 ). Memory for expectancy‐congruent and expectancy incongruent information: A review of the social and social developmental literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 42 – 61. |
|
dc.identifier |
Thorndyke, P. W. ( 1977 ). Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 77 – 110. |
|
dc.identifier |
Waddill, P. J., & McDaniel, M. A. ( 1998 ). Distinctiveness effects in recall: Differential processing or privileged retrieval Memory and Cognition, 26, 108 – 120. |
|
dc.identifier |
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. ( 2001 ). Meta‐analysis of theory‐of‐mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655 – 684. |
|
dc.identifier |
Alba, J. W., & Hasher, L. ( 1983 ). Is memory schematic Psychological Bulletin, 93, 203 – 231. |
|
dc.identifier |
Anderson, J. R., & Schooler, L. J. ( 1991 ). Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Science, 2, 396 – 408. |
|
dc.identifier |
Atran, S. ( 1998 ). Folkbiology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 547 – 609. |
|
dc.identifier |
Atran, S. ( 2002 ). In Gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. ( 2004 ). Religion's evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 713 – 770. |
|
dc.identifier |
Atran, S., & Sperber, D. ( 1991 ). Learning without teaching: Its place in culture. In L. T. Landsmann (Ed.), Culture, schooling, and psychological development (pp. 39 – 55 ). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. |
|
dc.identifier |
Avis, J., & Harris, P. L. ( 1991 ). Belief‐desire reasoning among Baka children. Child Development, 62, 460 – 467. |
|
dc.identifier |
Baillargeon, R. ( 1998 ). Infants' understanding of the physical world. In M. Sabourin, F. Craik, & M. Robert (Eds.), Advances in psychological science (Vol. 2, pp. 503 – 509 ). London: Psychology Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Barrett, J. L. ( 2000 ). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Science 4, 29 – 34. |
|
dc.identifier |
Barrett, J. L. ( 2004 ). Why would anyone believe in God ? Lanham, MD: AltaMira. |
|
dc.identifier |
Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. ( 1996 ). Conceptualizing a non‐natural entity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 219 – 247. |
|
dc.identifier |
Barrett, J. L., & Nyhof, M. A. ( 2001 ). Spreading nonnatural concepts: The role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 69 – 100. |
|
dc.identifier |
Bartlett, F. A. ( 1932 ). Remembering: A study in experimental psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Berger, J., & Heath, C. ( 2005 ). Idea habitats: How the prevalence of environmental cues influences the success of ideas. Cognitive Science, 29, 195 – 221. |
|
dc.identifier |
Berlin, B. ( 1992 ). Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Berlin, B., Breedlove, D., & Raven, P. ( 1973 ). General principles of classification and nomenclature in folk biology. American Anthropologist, 74, 214 – 242. |
|
dc.identifier |
Bower, G. H., Black, J. B., & Turner, T. J. ( 1979 ). Scripts in memory for text. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 177 – 220. |
|
dc.identifier |
Boyer, P. ( 1992 ). Explaining religious ideas: Outline of a cognitive approach. Numen, 39, 27 – 57. |
|
dc.identifier |
Boyer, P. ( 1994a ). Cognitive constraints on cultural representations: Natural ontologies and religious ideas. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 391 – 411 ). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Boyer, P. ( 1994b ). The naturalness of religious ideas. Berkeley: University of California Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Boyer, P. ( 2001 ). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books. |
|
dc.identifier |
Boyer, P. ( 2003 ). Religious thought and behaviour as by‐products of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 119 – 124. |
|
dc.identifier |
Boyer, P., & Ramble, C. ( 2001 ). Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross‐cultural evidence for recall of counter‐intuitive representations. Cognitive Science, 25, 535 – 564. |
|
dc.identifier |
Brewer, W. F. ( 1985 ). The story schema: Universal and culture‐specific properties. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Callahan, T., Rochat, P., Liilard, A., Claux, M., Odden, H., Itakura, S., et al. ( 2005 ). Synchrony in the onset of mental state reasoning: Evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science, 16, 378 – 384. |
|
dc.identifier |
Carey, S., & Spelke, E. ( 1994 ). Domain‐specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. ( 1972 ). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671 – 684. |
|
dc.identifier |
Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. ( 1975 ). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268 – 294. |
|
dc.identifier |
Davidson, D. ( 1994 ). Recognition and recall of irrelevant and interruptive atypical actions in script‐based stories. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 757 – 775. |
|
dc.identifier |
Flavell, J. H., Zhang, X.‐D., Zou, H., Dong, Q., & Qui, S. ( 1983 ). A comparison of the appearance‐reality distinction in the People's Republic of China and the United States. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 459 – 466. |
|
dc.identifier |
Gardner, D., Harris, P. L., Ohmoto, M., & Hamazaki, T. ( 1988 ). Japanese children's understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 203 – 218. |
|
dc.identifier |
Gelman, S. A., & Hirschfeld, L. A. ( 1998 ). How biological is essentialism ? In D. L. Medin & S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Heath, C., Bell, C., & Sternberg, E. ( 2001 ) Emotional selection in memes: The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1028 – 1041. |
|
dc.identifier |
Keil, F. ( 1989 ). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge: Bradford Book/MIT Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Keil, F. ( 1994 ). The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains: The origins of concepts of living things. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 234 – 254 ). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
|
dc.identifier |
Kelly, M. H., & Keil, F. ( 1985 ). The more things change … Metamorphoses and conceptual structure. Cognitive Science, 9, 403 – 416. |
|
dc.identifier.uri |
http://localhost:8080/xmlui/handle/CUHPOERS/100309 |
|
dc.description |
We hypothesize that cultural narratives such as myths and folktales are more likely to achieve cultural stability if they correspond to a minimally counterintuitive (MCI) cognitive template that includes mostly intuitive concepts combined with a minority of counterintuitive ones. Two studies tested this hypothesis, examining whether this template produces a memory advantage, and whether this memory advantage explains the cultural success of folktales. In a controlled laboratory setting, Study 1 found that an MCI template produces a memory advantage after a 1‐week delay, relative to entirely intuitive or maximally counterintuitive cognitive templates. Using archival methods, Study 2 examined the cognitive structure of Grimm Brothers folktales. Compared to culturally unsuccessful folktales, those that were demonstrably successful were especially likely to fit an MCI template. These findings highlight the role of human memory processes in cultural evolution. |
|
dc.description |
Peer Reviewed |
|
dc.description |
http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/98200/1/s15516709cog0000_68.pdf |
|
dc.format |
application/pdf |
|
dc.publisher |
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. |
|
dc.publisher |
Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
|
dc.rights |
IndexNoFollow |
|
dc.subject |
Concepts |
|
dc.subject |
Religion |
|
dc.subject |
Memory |
|
dc.subject |
Culture |
|
dc.subject |
Evolution |
|
dc.subject |
Neurosciences |
|
dc.subject |
Health Sciences |
|
dc.title |
Memory and Mystery: The Cultural Selection of Minimally Counterintuitive Narratives |
|
dc.type |
Article |
|